In recent years, the topic of police brutality in highly-policed communities has become more prevalent as over-policing continues to be pervasive in communities of color, especially Black communities, and with over-policing comes higher rates of instances of police brutality where the level of force being used is not justified by the situation. This is seen in the many videos documented by civilians which has brought this issue to national headlines and showed how pervasive this problem is as people continue to lose their lives. The use of body cameras in police departments have begun to rise as people begin to discuss methods of reform that will hold more officers accountable for their actions, or prevent as much police injustice as possible in order to improve civilian perceptions of police. However, the effectiveness of these body cameras is unclear, as police brutality continues to be an issue. Looking at previous literature on the topic, we intend to argue that due to various factors such as decreasing complaints about police in civilian-police encounters, public opinions on body-worn cameras, and the spillover effects of body-worn cameras in encounters, that their effect is positive in reducing civilian complaints, as public opinion about body-worn cameras in highly-policed communities also seems relatively positive.
How effective are body-worn cameras in decreasing police brutality in highly-policed communities?
In recent years, the topic of police brutality in highly-policed communities has become more prevalent as over-policing continues to be pervasive in communities of color, especially Black communities, and with over-policing comes higher rates of instances of police brutality where the level of force being used is not justified by the situation. This is seen in the many videos documented by civilians which has brought this issue to national headlines and showed how pervasive this problem is as people continue to lose their lives. The use of body cameras in police departments have begun to rise as people begin to discuss methods of reform that will hold more officers accountable for their actions, or prevent as much police injustice as possible in order to improve civilian perceptions of police. However, the effectiveness of these body cameras is unclear, as police brutality continues to be an issue. Looking at previous literature on the topic, we intend to argue that due to various factors such as decreasing complaints about police in civilian-police encounters, public opinions on body-worn cameras, and the spillover effects of body-worn cameras in encounters, that their effect is positive in reducing civilian complaints, as public opinion about body-worn cameras in highly-policed communities also seems relatively positive. To elaborate on this, Mummolo (2017) analyzes the effectiveness of modern policing tactics using millions of records of police-citizen interactions alongside officer interviews. He is evaluating the impact of a change to the protocol for stopping criminal suspects on police performance after the implementation of the procedure to report police reasons for stops, and which types of changes influence police behavior the most. He looked at weapon recovery rates during stops in order to investigate the reduction of unnecessary searches, as well as homicide rates after in relation to the reduction of stops. Through this, Mummolo discovered that procedural changes can quickly and dramatically alter officer behavior, suggesting that police reform strategies driven by psychological and personality-driven accounts are not as effective, as his research showed a correlation between the procedural change of reporting stops and the decreased amount of weapon recovery in stops which indicates it did in fact reduce unnecessary searches. We plan to build upon this research as we are looking at how to increase the effectiveness of policing tactics in administering the appropriate amount of force at the appropriate time. As Mummolo stated procedural changes are most effective at influencing police behavior, our research expands on this by specifically looking at police body-worn cameras (BWC) and whether this procedural change is effective enough in changing how officers treat citizens. Ariel et al. (2017) also addresses current research on civilian complaints about police, and then follows through with an analysis on the potential impacting factors behind BWCs on police-civilian encounters. Using police shifts as their unit of analysis, they look at results from a global, multisite randomized controlled trial on whether BWC use reduces citizens’ complaints. Its focus is more so on the effect of BWCs, stating that they found a reduction in complaints in pre–post comparisons, but found no significant differences when looking at post treatment comparisons or between the different sites. Our research builds on this as it goes more in-depth about the findings of the impacts of BWCs, factoring in another measurement besides complaints to give a more rigorous analysis on the influence of BWCs in civilian-police encounters. Extending on this article, we would be using other data, specifically in an area that is highly policed, to analyze how BWCs influence police-civilian encounters in highly-policed communities. Since this article gives a more rigorous analysis to their impacts, this would give us more information on how useful they are, and how they affect outcomes from these encounters. In a different article, the same researchers Ariel, Sutherland, and Henstock, et al. (2018) conducted a randomized control trial in ten different police departments. To collect data, they chose random and separate shifts where an officer would wear a body camera and another would not in order to see if the influence of self-awareness through body cameras affected their performance or influenced assaults against officers, which is what differs from their other work. They discovered that while body cameras increase individual chances of assault against an officer by 37%, overall in departments, assault was reduced by 61%. They also found that with officers who wore body cameras, complaints against officers reduced along with use of force and arrest rates. This is greatly connected with our research as it shows how force, arrests, and complaints reduce which is correlated with how we plan to measure police brutality as we will be focusing on complaints and use of force. We plan to expand on this by focusing on a highly-policed area, instead of a randomized sample, as this article does not take into account how factors such as the reported crime rates of an area may influence officers. The Braga, Barao, Zimmerman, et al. (2020) article examines the spillover effects of body-worn cameras (BWCs) in police-citizen encounters. It explains if BWCs have any significant impact on complaints from citizens, use of force from police, and police work activities such as arrests or officer-initiated calls. They are examining if any spillover effects from body-worn cameras have any influence over how citizens interact or behave with police officers in encounters with them. They found that BWCs do have statistical significance in reducing police complaints, there is no statistical significance on the effects of use of force while it does still reduce. Police activity saw no effects from BWCs, but it does deter both officers and citizens from inappropriate or illegal behaviors. Our research has to do with the effect of body-worn cameras in highly-policed communities, so our research will be building on this because it gives us a general idea of the outcomes of our research question, however we are also comparing racial differences in the amount of complaints and use of force. Our research will extend this article’s findings by conducting research on similar questions and hypotheses that are in line with the research question for this article. The Ray, Marsh, and Powelson (2017) article analyzes different perspectives on the effectiveness of BWCs from a civilian perspective through interviews. It takes into consideration the opinions from individuals in different racial groups (White, Black, Hispanic, Asian/Asian-American), and takes their perspectives and real-life experience with BWC into account. They found that respondents believed that BWC would either illuminate the difficulties of policing in support of the officer or it would create more transparency to hold officers more accountable for their actions in support of citizens. Our research question is about how effective BWCs are in highly-policed communities, and this will inevitably lead to different perspectives and opinions with how useful they actually are. This article is relevant because it shows us exactly what different members of different communities think of their effectiveness, which is one of the factors we’re looking for. Our research will extend this article by taking into account the interviewees’ experiences, and using that to conduct further research into the impact of BWCs in highly-policed communities, and how perceptions of BWCs change based on these lived experiences. We will also be comparing complaints among white and Black racial groups. The findings from the chosen research articles indicate that attitudes toward body-worn cameras in highly-policed communities have improved to a certain extent in recent years. Much of the literature concludes that there is a positive effect in reducing civilian complaints against police in encounters with them, and individuals from minoritized and highly-policed communities are in favor of them as they help promote police accountability. Yet multiple research studies validate the importance of looking more into police body-worn cameras. Therefore we plan to expand on existing research by focusing on a highly-policed community as our sample, and police injustice is being affected by body-worn cameras by looking at data before and after implementation of body-worn cameras. More specifically, our research question and data analysis will be examining how effective body-worn cameras are in decreasing civilian complaints and use of force in areas with higher rates of policing.
For our research question, we chose to focus on an existing dataset that studies a specific community. The dataset is from a large-scale field experiment with 2,224 officer-participants of the Metropolitan Police Department (MPD) in Washington, DC. This area is also one of the most highly policed areas in the country in regards to the officer-civilian ratio. They randomly assigned officers to receive cameras or not and tracked subsequent police behavior for 7 months using administrative data from the Metropolitan Police Department. The dataset is also separated into the seven districts that are policed by the MPD, and by officers wearing BWCs with 0 representing they are the control group and they are not wearing one, and 1 being the treatment. The column data within the set represents the number of each category that was reported by officers or recorded by the department’s administration There is already research published on the effect of body-worn cameras in different communities, and we believed it would be easier for us to collect information on one city or community rather than trying to compile data for the entire country. The data speaks to our research question because some of the key statistics that are measured are the number of body-worn cameras implemented in the police force (e.g. how many officers wear them). It also measures other statistics such as the rate of police-civilian encounters that may or may not involve violence, traffic stops, drugs, etc., and more importantly measurements of complaints against officers and reported uses of force, which are measurements we are trying to compute to answer our research question. Finally, it also speaks to our research question because the data on complaints is combined for overall complaints but also disaggregates into racial categories such as white, Black, and Hispanic, which can enable us to plot the data and see the trends of how effective (or ineffective) body-worn cameras are with factors such as race as well. While it will not be the main point, it is a point we want to examine. There is missing data in the datasource we plan on using, but we will only be focusing on the dataset that has an inputted number in every category, even though many numbers are at zero. For our methodology we used correlation and means to analyze the data. This is so that we can see if there is any association between the variables we’re looking at (body-worn camera effect and use of force, complaints, etc.). We also want to use means to compare the average rate of effectiveness of their implementation over time. We will interpret correlation coefficients to measure the strength of association between the two given variables we’re measuring. We will also be using averages to compute what general statistical significance there is to the variables we’re measuring, and if there’s any increase or decrease in averages and what that means for our research question. I think our analytical strategy can yield what we’re looking for, since we’re mostly looking for how strong the association is between the variables we’re measuring, and how averages in effects of different variables have changed over time.
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = all_complaints_post ~ Z + all_complaints_pre + block_id,
## data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -0.4810 -0.1636 -0.1627 -0.1389 5.8369
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 1.386e-01 1.646e-02 8.417 < 2e-16 ***
## Z 2.409e-02 2.069e-02 1.164 0.24447
## all_complaints_pre 5.177e-02 1.862e-02 2.780 0.00548 **
## block_id 7.740e-06 2.628e-05 0.294 0.76842
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.4832 on 2220 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.004199, Adjusted R-squared: 0.002853
## F-statistic: 3.12 on 3 and 2220 DF, p-value: 0.02507
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = use_of_force_1000_rate_pre ~ Z, data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -654.8 -654.8 -538.9 -538.9 21742.6
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 654.80 43.36 15.100 <2e-16 ***
## Z -115.95 59.31 -1.955 0.0507 .
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 1395 on 2222 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.001717, Adjusted R-squared: 0.001268
## F-statistic: 3.822 on 1 and 2222 DF, p-value: 0.05071
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = use_of_force_1000_rate_post ~ Z + use_of_force_1000_rate_pre +
## block_id, data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -7302.0 -618.6 -587.6 -498.8 22389.7
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 498.32303 62.38284 7.988 2.18e-15 ***
## Z 109.96895 77.33858 1.422 0.155
## use_of_force_1000_rate_pre 0.29979 0.02751 10.897 < 2e-16 ***
## block_id 0.08934 0.09818 0.910 0.363
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 1804 on 2220 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.05249, Adjusted R-squared: 0.05121
## F-statistic: 40.99 on 3 and 2220 DF, p-value: < 2.2e-16
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = use_of_force_black_pre ~ Z, data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -0.1892 -0.1892 -0.1865 -0.1865 4.8108
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 0.186473 0.016359 11.399 <2e-16 ***
## Z 0.002761 0.022373 0.123 0.902
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.5263 on 2222 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 6.855e-06, Adjusted R-squared: -0.0004432
## F-statistic: 0.01523 on 1 and 2222 DF, p-value: 0.9018
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = use_of_force_black_post ~ Z + use_of_force_black_pre +
## block_id, data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -1.2349 -0.2451 -0.2162 -0.2095 13.2927
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 2.127e-01 2.845e-02 7.476 1.10e-13 ***
## Z -5.803e-03 3.587e-02 -0.162 0.8715
## use_of_force_black_pre 2.038e-01 3.394e-02 6.006 2.22e-09 ***
## block_id 9.287e-05 4.569e-05 2.033 0.0422 *
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.8376 on 2220 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.01905, Adjusted R-squared: 0.01773
## F-statistic: 14.37 on 3 and 2220 DF, p-value: 2.835e-09
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = use_of_force_white_pre ~ Z, data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -0.01353 -0.01353 -0.00841 -0.00841 1.99159
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 0.013527 0.003345 4.044 5.43e-05 ***
## Z -0.005116 0.004574 -1.118 0.263
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.1076 on 2222 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.0005627, Adjusted R-squared: 0.0001129
## F-statistic: 1.251 on 1 and 2222 DF, p-value: 0.2635
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = use_of_force_white_post ~ Z + use_of_force_white_pre +
## block_id, data = officer)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -0.05427 -0.01723 -0.01448 -0.01270 1.98320
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 1.498e-02 3.992e-03 3.752 0.00018 ***
## Z 3.572e-03 5.104e-03 0.700 0.48413
## use_of_force_white_pre 2.620e-02 2.350e-02 1.115 0.26507
## block_id -1.670e-05 6.469e-06 -2.581 0.00992 **
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.1192 on 2220 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.003657, Adjusted R-squared: 0.002311
## F-statistic: 2.716 on 3 and 2220 DF, p-value: 0.04328
The results do not display any statistically significant relationship between the effects of body-worn cameras in police-civilian encounters, so we do not reject the null hypothesis. The graphs displayed show little to no complaints being filed against officers even with body-worn cameras recording the encounters and documenting what happens. However, one small difference that was noticed was there were slightly more complaints filed from Black and African-American people than there were from White people. Even if the data remains statistically insignificant, we believe it is still worth pointing out that by race there is a slight increase in complaints and use of force by race. The coefficients from data computation also did not indicate any kind of significant statistical relationship in the effectiveness of body-worn cameras with civilian-police encounters.
As previously stated, we do not reject the null hypothesis because the data is not statistically significant, but there are significant increases in the data based on race. We assumed that BWCs would reduce officer noncompliance with procedures or influence the demeanor of a suspect to lead to fewer complaints. However, because officers have the ability to turn off their cameras, which would eliminate the surveillance effect of BWCs, this could be a limitation in our research. Use of force is also self-reported by officers, which presents another limitation that could have influenced the data and potentially created a misrepresentation in the results. There was also a large number of zeros and single-digit numbers within our dataset, but the numbers would suddenly go into the thousands. This is most likely an inaccurate representation, however it was the most complete raw dataset we could find. Another limitation is the dataset is from Washington, D.C, so it is not representative of local areas, such as the San Diego County/LA County, or even California. We chose this dataset because the area is smaller, it has a smaller population, and is easier to collect raw data. It can be used as a rough benchmark for the effectiveness of body-worn cameras in civilian complaints, and use of force in civilian-police encounters. For further investigations, we intend to get a more complete dataset: to account for gender (e.g. white man vs. white woman, black woman vs. white woman, etc.), account for disability, and sexual orientation.
Mummolo, Jonathan. “Modern Police Tactics, Police-Citizen Interactions, and the Prospects for Reform.” The Journal of Politics, vol. 80, no. 1, Jan. 2018, pp. 1–15, doi:https://doi.org/10.1086/694393.
Ariel, Barak, et al. “‘Contagious Accountability.’” Criminal Justice and Behavior, vol. 44, no. 2, Sept. 2016, pp. 293–316, doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854816668218.
Ariel, Barak, Sutherland, A., Henstock, D. et al. “Paradoxical Effects of Self-Awareness of Being Observed: Testing the Effect of Police Body-Worn Cameras on Assaults and Aggression against Officers.” Journal of Experimental Criminology, vol. 14, no. 1, Dec. 2017, pp. 19–47, doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-017-9311-5.
Braga, Anthony A., et al. “Measuring the Direct and Spillover Effects of Body Worn Cameras on the Civility of Police–Citizen Encounters and Police Work Activities.” Journal of Quantitative Criminology, vol. 36, Oct. 2019, doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-019-09434-9.
Ray, Rashawn, et al. “Can Cameras Stop the Killings? Racial Differences in Perceptions of the Effectiveness of Body-Worn Cameras in Police Encounters,.” Sociological Forum, vol. 32, no. S1, July 2017, pp. 1032–50, doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/socf.12359.
Yokum, D. V., Ravishankar, A., Coppock, A., & Fieselmann, H. (2021, June 1). DC Body-Worn Camera Evaluation [officer_level_anon.csv]. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/P6VUH